Brain Detects Grammar Errors Subconsciously

Native speakers instinctively notice syntactical errors, but not ordinary "grammar errors."

Grammar Punctuation

A recent piece at ScienceDaily titled “Grammar Errors? The Brain Detects Them Even When You Are Unaware” caught my eye, given how many grammar errors I deal with on a daily basis. It turns out that they mean something different than I do by the term.

Participants in the study — native-English speaking people, ages 18-30 — had their brain activity recorded using electroencephalography, from which researchers focused on a signal known as the Event-Related Potential (ERP). This non-invasive technique allows for the capture of changes in brain electrical activity during an event. In this case, events were short sentences presented visually one word at a time.

Subjects were given 280 experimental sentences, including some that were syntactically (grammatically) correct and others containing grammatical errors, such as “We drank Lisa’s brandy by the fire in the lobby,” or “We drank Lisa’s by brandy the fire in the lobby.” A 50 millisecond audio tone was also played at some point in each sentence. A tone appeared before or after a grammatical faux pas was presented. The auditory distraction also appeared in grammatically correct sentences.

This approach, said lead author Laura Batterink, a postdoctoral researcher, provided a signature of whether awareness was at work during processing of the errors. “Participants had to respond to the tone as quickly as they could, indicating if its pitch was low, medium or high,” she said. “The grammatical violations were fully visible to participants, but because they had to complete this extra task, they were often not consciously aware of the violations. They would read the sentence and have to indicate if it was correct or incorrect. If the tone was played immediately before the grammatical violation, they were more likely to say the sentence was correct even it wasn’t.”

When tones appeared after grammatical errors, subjects detected 89 percent of the errors. In cases where subjects correctly declared errors in sentences, the researchers found a P600 effect, an ERP response in which the error is recognized and corrected on the fly to make sense of the sentence.

So, essentially, native speakers automatically detect nonsense sentences—phrases that simply don’t comport with the structure of the language they’ve been steeped in all their lives.  This is a far different thing from picking up ordinary “grammatical errors” such as confusing there, they’re, and their or its for it’s. For that matter, it’s likely why even extremely competent writers violate made-up rules like “don’t split infinitives” or “don’t end sentences in prepositions”—doing those things sounds natural.

I’ve read that master chess players can catch a brief glimpse of a game board and reconstruct it exactly; they can’t, however, do this if the pieces are simply arranged randomly on the board.

FILED UNDER: Uncategorized,
James Joyner
About James Joyner
James Joyner is Professor and Department Head of Security Studies at Marine Corps University's Command and Staff College. He's a former Army officer and Desert Storm veteran. Views expressed here are his own. Follow James on Twitter @DrJJoyner.

Comments

  1. OzarkHillbilly says:

    So, essentially, native speakers automatically detect nonsense sentences—phrases that simply don’t comport with the structure of the language they’ve been stepped in all their life. This is a far different thing from picking up ordinary “grammatical errors” such as confusing there, they’re, and their or its for it’s.

    Could be because as we are writing we are saying the words in our minds and if they sound right, we don’t pick it up. Than again, maybe were just stupid. 😉

  2. Justinian says:

    It takes a controlled experiment to tell us this?

    Anyone who has learned a foreign language knows what a huge amount of intellectual effort is expended in parsing grammar, because the person must process the grammar of the foreign language through conscious channels. Since native speakers certainly don’t go through all that mental effort consciously, and yet speak, read, and write in a grammatically correct way (mostly correct, anyway), obviously all that processing occurs subconsciously.

  3. rachel says:

    …phrases that simply don’t comport with the structure of the language they’ve been stepped in all their life.

    Ahem. And “life” should be “lives”.

  4. EMRVentures says:

    I assume all this research is off the table when it comes to blog commenting, where few enough people appear to even possess a native language.

  5. Andre Kenji says:

    @Justinian:

    Anyone who has learned a foreign language knows what a huge amount of intellectual effort is expended in parsing grammar, because the person must process the grammar of the foreign language through conscious channels.

    Not necessarily. I write in English as a Second Language, and I always commit errors, both in Grammar and in Style(Sometimes I write in English using a Latin Structure, sometimes I write sentences and phrases that only makes sense in Portuguese, and so on).

    To learn a Foreign Language you have to learn how the language works, not Grammar. But yes, I´m less aggressive writing in English than in my native Language.